Want to share your content on R-bloggers? click here if you have a blog, or here if you don't.
Way back in 2018, long before the pandemic, I described a soon-to-be implemented simstudy
function genMultiFac
that facilitates the generation of multi-factorial study data. I followed up that post with a description of how we can use these types of efficient designs to answer multiple questions in the context of a single study.
Fast forward three years, and I am thinking about these designs again for a new grant application that proposes to study simultaneously three interventions aimed at reducing emergency department (ED) use for people living with dementia. The primary interest is to evaluate each intervention on its own terms, but also to assess whether any combinations seem to be particularly effective. While this will be a fairly large cluster randomized trial with about 80 EDs being randomized to one of the 8 possible combinations, I was concerned about our ability to estimate the interaction effects of multiple interventions with sufficient precision to draw useful conclusions, particularly if the combined effects of two or three interventions are less than additive. (That is, two interventions may be better than one, but not twice as good.)
I am thinking that a null hypothesis testing framework might not be so useful here, given the that the various estimates could be highly uncertain, not to mention the multiple statistical tests that we would need to conduct (and presumably adjust for). Rather, a Bayesian approach that pools estimates across interventions and provides posterior probability distributions may provide more insight into how the interventions interact could be a better way to go.
With this in mind, I went to the literature, and I found these papers by Kassler et al and Gelman. They both describe a way of thinking about interaction that emphasizes the estimates of variance across effect estimands. I went ahead and tested the idea with simulated data, which I’m showing here. Ultimately, I decided that this approach will not work so well for our study, and I came up with a pretty simple solution that I will share next time.
Identifying interaction through variance
The scenarios described by both papers involve studies that may be evaluating many possible interventions or exposures, each of which may have two or more levels. If we are dealing with a normally distributed (continuous) outcome measure, we can model that outcome as
\[ y_{i} \sim N\left(\mu = \tau_0 + \tau^1_{j_{1_i}} + \dots + \tau^k_{j_{k_i}} + \tau^{12}_{j_{12_i}} + \dots + \tau^{k-1, k}_{j_{k-1,k_i}} + \tau^{123}_{123_i} + \dots + \tau^{k-2, k-1, k}_{k-2, k-1, k_i} + \dots, \ \sigma = \sigma_0\right), \]
where there are \(K\) interventions, and intervention \(k\) has \(j_k\) levels. So, if intervention \(3\) has 4 levels, \(j_3 \in \{1,2,3,4\}.\) \(\tau_0\) is effectively the grand mean. \(\tau^k_1, \tau^k_2, \dots, \tau^k_{j_k},\) are the mean contributions for the \(k\)th intervention, and we constrain \(\sum_{m=1}^{j_k} \tau^k_m = 0.\) Again, for intervention \(3\), we would have \(\tau^3_1 \dots, \tau^3_4,\) with \(\sum_{m=1}^{4} \tau^3_m = 0.\)
The adjustments made for the two-way interactions are represented by the \(\tau^{12}\)’s through the \(\tau^{k-1,k}\)’s. If intervention 5 has \(2\) levels then for the interaction between interventions 3 and 5 we have \(\tau^{35}_{11}, \tau^{35}_{12}, \tau^{35}_{21}, \dots, \tau^{35}_{42}\) and \(\sum_{m=1}^4 \sum_{n=1}^2 \tau^{35}_{m,n} = 0.\)
This pattern continues for higher orders of interaction (i.e. 3-way, 4-way, etc.).
In the Bayesian model, each set of \(\tau_k\)’s shares a common prior distribution with mean 0 and standard deviation \(\sigma_k\):
\[ \tau^k_1, \dots, \tau^k_{j_k} \sim N(\mu = 0, \sigma = \sigma_k), \] where \(\sigma_k\) is a hyperparameter that will be estimated from the data. The same is true for the interaction terms for interventions \(k\) and \(l\):
\[ \tau^{kl}_{11}, \dots, \tau^{kl}_{j_k, j_l} \sim N(\mu = 0, \sigma = \sigma_{kl}), \ \ \text{where } k < l \]
To assess whether there is interaction between the interventions (i.e. the effects are not merely additive), we are actually interested the variance parameters of the interaction \(\tau\text{'s}\). If, for example there is no interaction between different levels of interventions of 3 and 5, then \(\sigma_{35}\) should be close to \(0\), implying that \(\tau^{35}_{11} \approx \tau^{35}_{12} \approx \dots \approx \tau^{35}_{42} \approx 0\). On the other hand, if there is some interaction effect, then \(\sigma_{35} > 0,\) implying that at least one \(\tau^{35} > 0.\)
One advantage of the proposed Bayesian model is that we can use partial pooling to get more precise estimates of the variance terms. By this, I mean that we can use information from each \(\sigma^{kl}\) to inform the others. So, in the case of 2-way interaction, the prior probability assumption would suggest that the the variance terms were drawn from a common distribution:
\[ \sigma^{12}, \sigma^{13}, \dots, \sigma^{k-1,k} \sim N(\mu = 0, \sigma = \sigma_{\text{2-way}}) \]
We can impose more structure (and hopefully precision) by doing the same for the main effects:
\[ \sigma^{1}, \sigma^{2}, \dots, \sigma^{k} \sim N(\mu = 0, \sigma = \sigma_{\text{main}}) \]
Of course, for each higher order interaction (above 2-way), we could impose the same structure:
\[ \sigma^{123}, \dots, \sigma^{12k}, \dots, \sigma^{k-2, k-1, k} \sim N(\mu = 0, \sigma = \sigma_{\text{3-way}}) \]
And so on. Though at some point, we might want to assume that there is no higher order interaction and exclude it from the model; in most cases, we could stop at 2- or 3-way interaction and probably not sacrifice too much.
Example from simulation
When I set out to explore this model, I started relatively simple, using only two interventions with four levels each. In this case, the factorial study would have 16 total arms \((4 \times 4)\). (Since I am using only 2 interventions, I am changing the notation slightly, using interventions \(a\) and \(b\) rather than \(1\) and \(2\).) Individual \(i\) is randomized to one level in \(a\) and one level \(b\), and \(a_i \in \{1,2,3,4\}\) and \(b_i\in \{1,2,3,4\}\), and \(ab_i \in \{11, 12, 13, 14, 21, 22, \dots, 44\}.\) Using the same general model from above, here is the specific model for continuous \(y\):
\[ y_{i} \sim N\left(\mu = \tau_0 + \tau^a_{a_i} + \tau^b_{b_i} + \tau^{ab}_{ab_i}, \ \sigma = \sigma_0\right) \]
Take note that we only have a single set of 2-way interactions since there are only two groups of interventions. Because of this, there is no need for a \(\sigma_{\text{2-way}}\) hyperparameter; however, there is a hyperparameter \(\sigma_{\text{main}}\) to pool across the main effects of \(a\) and \(b\). Here are the prior distribution assumptions:
\[\begin{aligned} \tau_0 &\sim N(0, 5) \\ \tau^a_1, \tau^a_2, \tau^a_3, \tau^a_4 &\sim N(0, \sigma_a) \\ \tau^b_1, \tau^b_2, \tau^b_3, \tau^b_4 &\sim N(0, \sigma_b) \\ \tau^{ab}_{11}, \tau^{ab}_{12}, \dots \tau^{ab}_{44} &\sim N(0, \sigma_{ab}) \\ \sigma_a, \sigma_b &\sim N(0, \sigma_\text{main}) \\ \sigma_{ab} &\sim N(0, 5) \\ \sigma_\text{main} &\sim N(0, 5) \\ \sigma &\sim N(0,5) \end{aligned}\]In order to ensure identifiability, we have the following constraints:
\[\begin{aligned} \tau^a_1 + \tau^a_2 + \tau^a_3 + \tau^a_4 &= 0 \\ \tau^b_1 + \tau^b_2 + \tau^b_3 + \tau^b_4 &= 0 \\ \tau^{ab}_{11} + \tau^{ab}_{12} + \dots + \tau^{ab}_{43} + \tau^{ab}_{44} &= 0 \end{aligned}\]Required libraries
library(simstudy) library(data.table) library(cmdstanr) library(caret) library(posterior) library(bayesplot) library(ggdist) library(glue)
Data generation
The parameters \(\tau_0, \tau_a, \tau_b, \text{ and } \tau_{ab}\) are set so that there is greater variation in treatment \(a\) compared to treatment \(b\). In both cases, the sum of the parameters is set to \(0\).
t_0 <- 0 t_a <- c(-8, -1, 3, 6) t_b <- c(-3, -1, 0, 4)
The interaction is set in this case so that there is an added effect when both \(a=2 \ \& \ b=2\) and \(a=3 \ \& \ b=2\). Again, the parameters are set so that the sum-to-zero constraint is maintained.
x <- c(4, 3) nox <- - sum(x) / (16 - length(x)) t_ab <- matrix(c(nox, nox, nox, nox, nox, 4, nox, nox, nox, 3, nox, nox, nox, nox, nox, nox), nrow = 4, byrow = TRUE) t_ab ## [,1] [,2] [,3] [,4] ## [1,] -0.5 -0.5 -0.5 -0.5 ## [2,] -0.5 4.0 -0.5 -0.5 ## [3,] -0.5 3.0 -0.5 -0.5 ## [4,] -0.5 -0.5 -0.5 -0.5 sum(t_ab) ## [1] 0
The data definitions for the arm assignments and the outcome \(y\) are established using the simstudy
package:
d1 <- defDataAdd(varname = "y", formula = "mu", variance = 16, dist = "normal")
Now we are ready to generate the data:
set.seed(110) dd <- genMultiFac(nFactors = 2, levels = 4, each = 30, colNames = c("a", "b")) dd[, mu := t_0 + t_a[a] + t_b[b] + t_ab[a, b], keyby = id] dd <- addColumns(d1, dd)
Plot of \(\bar{y}\) by arm
The plot shows the the average outcomes by arm. The interaction when \(a=2 \ \& \ b=2\) and \(a=3 \ \& \ b=2\) is apparent in the two locations where the smooth pattern of increases is interrupted.
Sampling from the posterior
The function shown next simply generates the data needed by Stan
. (The Stan
implementation is shown below in the addendum.) Take note that we convert the \(\tau_{ab}\) design matrix of 0’s and 1’s to a single vector with values ranging from 1 to 16.
dt_to_list <- function(dx) { dx[, a_f := factor(a)] dx[, b_f := factor(b)] dv <- dummyVars(~ b_f:a_f , data = dx, n = c(4, 4)) dp <- predict(dv, dx ) N <- nrow(dx) ## number of observations I <- 2 X2 <- 1 main <- as.matrix(dx[,.(a,b)]) ab <- as.vector(dp %*% c(1:16)) x <- as.matrix(ab, nrow = N, ncol = X2) y <- dx[, y] list(N=N, I=I, X2=X2, main=main, x=x, y=y) }
I am using cmdstanr
to interact with Stan
:
mod <- cmdstan_model("code/model_2_factors.stan", force_recompile = TRUE) fit <- mod$sample( data = dt_to_list(dd), refresh = 0, chains = 4L, parallel_chains = 4L, iter_warmup = 500, iter_sampling = 2500, adapt_delta = 0.99, step_size = .05, max_treedepth = 20, seed = 1721 ) ## Running MCMC with 4 parallel chains... ## ## Chain 2 finished in 15.5 seconds. ## Chain 1 finished in 17.7 seconds. ## Chain 3 finished in 20.3 seconds. ## Chain 4 finished in 23.3 seconds. ## ## All 4 chains finished successfully. ## Mean chain execution time: 19.2 seconds. ## Total execution time: 23.6 seconds.
Diagnostic checks
Here is just one set of trace plots for \(\tau^a_1, \dots, \tau^a_4\) that indicate the sampling went quite well – the variables not shown were equally well-behaved.
posterior <- as_draws_array(fit$draws()) mcmc_trace(posterior, pars = glue("t[{1},{1:4}]"))
Variance estimates
Since we are focused on the possibility of 2-way interaction, the primary parameter of interest is \(\sigma_{ab},\) the variation of the interaction effects. (In the Stan
model specification this variance parameter is sigma_x, as in interaction.) The plot shows the 95% credible intervals for each of the main effect variance parameters as well as the interaction variance parameter.
The fact that the two main effect variance parameters (\(\sigma_a\) and \(\sigma_b\)) are greater than zero supports the data generation process which assumed different outcomes for different levels of interventions \(a\) and \(b\), respectively.
And the credible interval for \(\sigma_{ab}\) (sigma_x), likewise is shifted away from zero, suggesting there might be some interaction between \(a\) and \(b\) at certain levels of each.
mcmc_intervals(posterior, pars = c(glue("sigma_m[{1:2}]"), "sigma_x[1]"))
We can hone in a bit more on the specific estimates of the \(\tau_{ab}\)’s to see where those interactions might be occurring. It appears that t_x[1,6] (representing \(\tau_{22}\)) is an important interaction term – which is consistent with the data generation process. However, \(\tau_{32}\), represented by t_x[1,10] is not obviously important. Perhaps we need more data.
mcmc_intervals(posterior, pars = glue("t_x[1,{1:16}]"))
Below is a visual representation of how well the model fits the data by showing the interval of predicted cell counts for each \(a/b\) pair. The observed means (shown as white dots) sit on top of the predictions (shown by the colored lines), suggesting the model is appropriate.
r <- as_draws_rvars(fit$draws(variables = c("t_0","t", "t_x"))) dnew <- data.frame( genMultiFac(nFactors = 2, levels = 4, each = 1, colNames = c("b", "a"))) dnew$yhat <- with(r, rep(t_0, 16) + rep(t[1, ], each = 4) + rep(t[2, ], times = 4) + t(t_x)) ggplot(data = dnew, aes(x=b, dist = yhat)) + geom_vline(aes(xintercept = b), color = "white", size = .25) + stat_dist_lineribbon() + geom_point(data = dsum, aes(y = yhat), color = "white", size = 2) + facet_grid(.~a, labeller = labeller(a = label_both)) + theme(panel.grid.minor = element_blank(), panel.grid.major = element_blank()) + scale_fill_brewer()
Only one treatment effect and no interaction
Perhaps the rationale for focusing on the variance can be best appreciated by looking at a contrasting scenario where there is only a single main effect (for intervention \(a\)) and no interaction. Here we would expect the estimates for the intervention \(b\) main effects variance as well as the variance of the interaction terms to be close to zero.
t_0 <- 0 t_a <- c(-8, -1, 3, 6) t_b <- c(0, 0, 0, 0) t_ab <- matrix(0, nrow = 4, ncol = 4)
The plot of the observed means is consistent with the data generation process:
## Running MCMC with 4 parallel chains... ## ## Chain 2 finished in 9.4 seconds. ## Chain 1 finished in 9.9 seconds. ## Chain 4 finished in 10.2 seconds. ## Chain 3 finished in 21.8 seconds. ## ## All 4 chains finished successfully. ## Mean chain execution time: 12.8 seconds. ## Total execution time: 21.9 seconds.
And yes, the posterior distribution for \(\sigma_{ab}\) (sigma_x) is now very close to zero …
and the effect parameters are all centered around zero:
Once again, the predicted values are quite close to the observed means – indicating the model is a good fit:
Next steps
In the motivating application, there are actually three interventions, but each one has only two levels (yes or no). In this case, the level mean and across-level variance parameters were poorly estimated, probably because there are so few levels. This forced me to take a more traditional approach, where I estimate the means of each randomization arm. I’ll share that next time.
< color="darkkhaki">
References:
Gelman, Andrew. “Analysis of variance—why it is more important than ever.” The annals of statistics 33, no. 1 (2005): 1-53.
Kassler, Daniel, Ira Nichols-Barrer, and Mariel Finucane. “Beyond “treatment versus control”: How Bayesian analysis makes factorial experiments feasible in education research.” Evaluation review 44, no. 4 (2020): 238-261.
< >
Addendum
The model is implemented in Stan using a non-centered parameterization, so that the parameters \(tau\) are a function of a set of \(z\) parameters, which are standard normal parameters. This does not dramatically change the estimates, but eliminates divergent chains, improving sampling behavior.
data { int<lower=1> N; // # of observations int<lower=1> I; // # of interventions int<lower=1> X2; // # of 2-way interactions int main[N, I]; // interventions int x[N, X2]; // interactions vector[N] y; // outcome } parameters { real t_0; vector[3] z_raw[I]; vector[15] z_x_raw[X2]; real<lower=0> sigma; real<lower=0> sigma_m[I]; real<lower=0> sigma_x[X2]; real<lower=0> sigma_main; } transformed parameters { // constrain parameters to sum to 0 vector[4] z[I]; vector[16] z_x[X2]; vector[4] t[I]; vector[16] t_x[X2]; vector[N] yhat; for (i in 1:I) { z[i] = append_row(z_raw[i], -sum(z_raw[i])); } for (i in 1:X2) { z_x[i] = append_row(z_x_raw[i], -sum(z_x_raw[i])); } for (i in 1:I) for (j in 1:4) t[i, j] = sigma_m[i] * z[i, j]; for (i in 1:X2) for (j in 1:16) t_x[i, j] = sigma_x[i] * z_x[i, j]; // yhat for (n in 1:N) { real ytemp; ytemp = t_0; for (i in 1:I) ytemp = ytemp + t[i, main[n, i]]; // 2 sets of main effects for (i in 1:X2) ytemp = ytemp + t_x[i, x[n, i]]; // 1 set of interaction effects yhat[n] = ytemp; } } model { sigma ~ normal(0, 5); sigma_m ~ normal(0, sigma_main); sigma_x ~ normal(0, 5); sigma_main ~ normal(0, 5); t_0 ~ normal(0, 5); for (i in 1:I) z_raw[i] ~ std_normal(); for (i in 1:X2) z_x_raw[i] ~ std_normal(); y ~ normal(yhat, sigma); }
R-bloggers.com offers daily e-mail updates about R news and tutorials about learning R and many other topics. Click here if you're looking to post or find an R/data-science job.
Want to share your content on R-bloggers? click here if you have a blog, or here if you don't.