Want to share your content on R-bloggers? click here if you have a blog, or here if you don't.
In this post, I demonstrate a few techniques for plotting information from a relatively simple mixed-effects model fit in R. These plots can help us develop intuitions about what these models are doing and what “partial pooling” means.
The sleepstudy
dataset
For these examples, I’m going to use the sleepstudy
dataset from the lme4
package. The outcome measure is reaction time, the predictor measure is days of
sleep deprivation, and these measurements are nested within participants—we
have 10 observations per participant. I am also going to add two fake
participants with incomplete data to illustrate partial pooling.
library(lme4) #> Loading required package: Matrix #> Loading required package: methods library(dplyr) library(tibble) # Convert to tibble for better printing. Convert factors to strings sleepstudy <- sleepstudy %>% as_tibble() %>% mutate(Subject = as.character(Subject)) # Add two fake participants df_sleep <- bind_rows( sleepstudy, data_frame(Reaction = c(286, 288), Days = 0:1, Subject = "374"), data_frame(Reaction = 245, Days = 0, Subject = "373")) df_sleep #> # A tibble: 183 x 3 #> Reaction Days Subject #> <dbl> <dbl> <chr> #> 1 249.5600 0 308 #> 2 258.7047 1 308 #> 3 250.8006 2 308 #> 4 321.4398 3 308 #> 5 356.8519 4 308 #> 6 414.6901 5 308 #> 7 382.2038 6 308 #> 8 290.1486 7 308 #> 9 430.5853 8 308 #> 10 466.3535 9 308 #> # ... with 173 more rows
We can visualize all the data in ggplot2 by using facet_wrap()
to create
subplots for each participant and stat_smooth()
to create a regression line
in each subplot.
library(ggplot2) xlab <- "Days of sleep deprivation" ylab <- "Average reaction time (ms)" ggplot(df_sleep) + aes(x = Days, y = Reaction) + stat_smooth(method = "lm", se = FALSE) + # Put the points on top of lines geom_point() + facet_wrap("Subject") + labs(x = xlab, y = ylab) + # We also need to help the x-axis, so it doesn't # create gridlines/ticks on 2.5 days scale_x_continuous(breaks = 0:4 * 2)
By the way, ggplot2 doesn’t draw the regressions lines outside of the range of
the data unless we set fullrange = TRUE
. That’s a helpful feature for 374!
Complete pooling and no pooling models
Each one of these panels plotted above shows an independently estimated regression line. This approach to fitting a separate line for each participant is sometimes called the no pooling model because none of the information from different participants is combined or pooled together.
We fit a separate line for each cluster of data, unaware
that any of the other participants exist. The lmList()
function in lme4
automates this process.
df_no_pooling <- lmList(Reaction ~ Days | Subject, df_sleep) %>% coef() %>% # Subject IDs are stored as row-names. Make them an explicit column rownames_to_column("Subject") %>% rename(Intercept = `(Intercept)`, Slope_Days = Days) %>% add_column(Model = "No pooling") %>% # Remove the participant who only had one data-point filter(Subject != "373") head(df_no_pooling) #> Subject Intercept Slope_Days Model #> 1 308 244.1927 21.764702 No pooling #> 2 309 205.0549 2.261785 No pooling #> 3 310 203.4842 6.114899 No pooling #> 4 330 289.6851 3.008073 No pooling #> 5 331 285.7390 5.266019 No pooling #> 6 332 264.2516 9.566768 No pooling
In contrast, we might consider a complete pooling model where all the information from the participants is combined together. We fit a single line for the combined data set, unaware that the data came from different participants.
# Fit a model on all the data pooled together m_pooled <- lm(Reaction ~ Days, df_sleep) # Repeat the intercept and slope terms for each participant df_pooled <- data_frame( Model = "Complete pooling", Subject = unique(df_sleep$Subject), Intercept = coef(m_pooled)[1], Slope_Days = coef(m_pooled)[2]) head(df_pooled) #> # A tibble: 6 x 4 #> Model Subject Intercept Slope_Days #> <chr> <chr> <dbl> <dbl> #> 1 Complete pooling 308 252.3207 10.32766 #> 2 Complete pooling 309 252.3207 10.32766 #> 3 Complete pooling 310 252.3207 10.32766 #> 4 Complete pooling 330 252.3207 10.32766 #> 5 Complete pooling 331 252.3207 10.32766 #> 6 Complete pooling 332 252.3207 10.32766
We can compare these two approaches. Instead of calculating the regression lines
with stat_smooth()
, we can use geom_abline()
to draw the lines from our
dataframe of intercept and slope parameters.
# Join the raw data so we can use plot the points and the lines. df_models <- bind_rows(df_pooled, df_no_pooling) %>% left_join(df_sleep, by = "Subject") p_model_comparison <- ggplot(df_models) + aes(x = Days, y = Reaction) + # Set the color mapping in this layer so the points don't get a color geom_abline(aes(intercept = Intercept, slope = Slope_Days, color = Model), size = .75) + geom_point() + facet_wrap("Subject") + labs(x = xlab, y = ylab) + scale_x_continuous(breaks = 0:4 * 2) + # Fix the color palette scale_color_brewer(palette = "Dark2") + theme(legend.position = "top") p_model_comparison
If we stare at this plot, a few things become apparent. The complete pooling model estimates a single line, and we see that same line drawn on every facet. One advantage is that the model can make a guess about the line for 373 who only has one observation. That model looks pretty terrible elsewhere—309, 310, etc.—because nobody is perfectly average. In contrast, the no pooling model can follow the data, fitting the sharp trend upwards in 308 and even capturing the negative slope in 335.
(Here’s a fun question: Which approach has the better guess for 374’s line?)
The no pooling model cannot make a guess about 373. In Statistical Rethinking, McElreath says these models have amnesia :hushed::
Many statistical models also have anterograde amnesia. As the models move from one cluster—individual, group, location—in the data to another, estimating parameters for each cluster, they forget everything about the previous clusters. They behave this way, because the assumptions force them to. Any of the models from previous chapters that used dummy variables to handle categories are programmed for amnesia. These models implicitly assume that nothing learned about any one category informs estimates for the other categories—the parameters are independent of one another and learn from completely separate portions of the data. This would be like forgetting you had ever been in a café, each time you go to a new café. Cafés do differ, but they are also alike.
Once the no pooling model draws the line for 372, and it completely forgets everything it has seen and moves on to 373. It has to skip 373 because it cannot estimate a line from a single point, and it moves on.
Improving estimates with a mixed-effects model
We can do better with mixed-effects models. In these models, we pool information from all the lines together to improve our estimates of each individual line. This approach is sometimes called partial pooling. In particular, after seeing the 18 trend lines for the participants with complete data, we can make an informed guess about the trend lines for the two participants with incomplete data.
We can fit a classical mixed-effects model with the lme4 package:
m <- lmer(Reaction ~ 1 + Days + (1 + Days | Subject), df_sleep) arm::display(m) #> lmer(formula = Reaction ~ 1 + Days + (1 + Days | Subject), data = df_sleep) #> coef.est coef.se #> (Intercept) 252.54 6.43 #> Days 10.45 1.54 #> #> Error terms: #> Groups Name Std.Dev. Corr #> Subject (Intercept) 24.14 #> Days 5.92 0.07 #> Residual 25.48 #> --- #> number of obs: 183, groups: Subject, 20 #> AIC = 1783.4, DIC = 1787.8 #> deviance = 1779.6
The first two coef.est
items are the “fixed effects” estimates; they reflect
the average intercept and slope parameters. For this example, the values are
practically the same as the complete-pooling estimates. This model assumes that
each participant’s individual intercept and slope parameters are deviations
from this average, and these random deviations drawn from a distribution of
possible intercept and slope parameters. These are “randomly varying” or “random
effects”. The information in the Error terms
area describes the distribution
of the effects. Because we have both fixed and random effects,
we have a “mixed-effects” model. Hence the name.
To visualize these estimates, we extract each participant’s intercept and slope
using coef()
.
# Make a dataframe with the fitted effects df_partial_pooling <- coef(m)[["Subject"]] %>% as_tibble() %>% rownames_to_column("Subject") %>% rename(Intercept = `(Intercept)`, Slope_Days = Days) %>% mutate(Model = "Partial pooling") head(df_partial_pooling) #> # A tibble: 6 x 4 #> Subject Intercept Slope_Days Model #> <chr> <dbl> <dbl> <chr> #> 1 308 253.9478 19.626420 Partial pooling #> 2 309 211.7334 1.731866 Partial pooling #> 3 310 213.1585 4.906097 Partial pooling #> 4 330 275.1422 5.643646 Partial pooling #> 5 331 273.7283 7.386313 Partial pooling #> 6 332 260.6503 10.163271 Partial pooling
Update the previous plot with a dataframe of all three models’ estimates.
df_models <- bind_rows(df_pooled, df_no_pooling, df_partial_pooling) %>% left_join(df_sleep, by = "Subject") # Replace the data-set of the last plot p_model_comparison %+% df_models
Most of the time, the no pooling and partial pooling lines are on top of each other. But when the two differ, it’s because the partial pooling model’s line is pulled slightly towards the no-pooling line.
We can appreciate the differences by zooming in on some participants.
df_zoom <- df_models %>% filter(Subject %in% c("335", "350", "373", "374")) p_model_comparison %+% df_zoom
The negative line for 335 from the no pooling model gets a flatter slope in the partial pooling model. The model knows that negative trends are rather unlikely, so the it hedges its bets and pulls that line towards the group average. Something similar happens with 350 where a sharp slope is slightly attenuated. For the participants with incomplete data, the partial pooling model is much more like the complete pooling model. The complete pooling and the partial pooling lines are basically parallel—i.e, they have the same slope. That’s a reasonable guess given so little information.
It’s shrinkage
The partial pooling model pulls more extreme estimates towards an overall average. We can visualize this effect by plotting a scatterplot of intercept and slope parameters from each model and connecting estimates for the same participant.
# Also visualize the point for the fixed effects df_fixef <- data_frame( Model = "Partial pooling (average)", Intercept = fixef(m)[1], Slope_Days = fixef(m)[2]) # Complete pooling / fixed effects are center of gravity in the plot df_gravity <- df_pooled %>% distinct(Model, Intercept, Slope_Days) %>% bind_rows(df_fixef) df_gravity #> # A tibble: 2 x 3 #> Model Intercept Slope_Days #> <chr> <dbl> <dbl> #> 1 Complete pooling 252.3207 10.32766 #> 2 Partial pooling (average) 252.5426 10.45212 df_pulled <- bind_rows(df_no_pooling, df_partial_pooling) ggplot(df_pulled) + aes(x = Intercept, y = Slope_Days, color = Model) + geom_point(size = 2) + geom_point(data = df_gravity, size = 5) + # Draw an arrow connecting the observations between models geom_path(aes(group = Subject, color = NULL), arrow = arrow(length = unit(.02, "npc"))) + # Use ggrepel to jitter the labels away from the points ggrepel::geom_text_repel( aes(label = Subject, color = NULL), data = df_no_pooling) + # Don't forget 373 ggrepel::geom_text_repel( aes(label = Subject, color = NULL), data = filter(df_partial_pooling, Subject == "373")) + theme(legend.position = "bottom") + ggtitle("Pooling of regression parameters") + xlab("Intercept estimate") + ylab("Slope estimate") + scale_color_brewer(palette = "Dark2")
The average intercept and slope act like a center of gravity, pulling values parameter estimates towards it. Hmm, maybe gravity is not quite the right analogy, because the pull is greater for more extreme values. The lines near that center point are very short; they get adjusted very little. The lines in general get longer as we move away from the complete pooling estimate. The fewer the observations in a cluster (here, participants), the more information is borrowed from other clusters, and the greater the pull towards the average estimate. Participant 373 had one observation, so their slope estimate is the average. Likewise, 374 had only two observations, so they get pulled the farthest and receive a slope estimate near the overall average.
This effect is sometimes called shrinkage, because more extreme values shrinkage are pulled towards a more reasonable, more average value. In the lme4 book, Douglas Bates provides an alternative to shrinkage:
The term “shrinkage” may have negative connotations. John Tukey preferred to refer to the process as the estimates for individual subjects “borrowing strength” from each other. This is a fundamental difference in the models underlying mixed-effects models versus strictly fixed effects models. In a mixed-effects model we assume that the levels of a grouping factor are a selection from a population and, as a result, can be expected to share characteristics to some degree. Consequently, the predictions from a mixed-effects model are attenuated relative to those from strictly fixed-effects models.
Shrinkage, borrowing strength :muscle: … Another term would also be regularization if we think about how the model avoids overfitting by the taming extreme estimates.
This feature is why I use mixed effects models in my work. If I have a speech discrimination experiment and I want to describe a child’s speech perception ability, I am going to use the partially pooled, shrunken, strength-borrowing, regularized, model-derived estimate of their ability, because it uses more information. It’s that simple to me. Other disciplines might highlight other reasons to use these models, but for me, it’s partially pooling information that’s the most attractive feature.
A topographic map of parameters
For the next visualization, we are going to visualize the distribution of randomly varying effects. Honestly, I am partly including it just so that I have a working ggplot2 version of how to make this plot online. It’s not a routine visualization, but it reveals a little more about where estimates are being pulled towards.
I already remarked that the model estimates a distribution of intercept and slope effects. We know where the center of that distribution is: It’s the fixed effects estimate, the center of gravity in the last plot. What the model also needs to estimate is the variability/spread of values around that center. Also, intercepts and slopes might be correlated: Maybe the effect of an additional day on reaction time is diminished for participants who are slower to respond in general. So, the model also estimates the correlation of those effects too.
Imagine that the last plot is a landscape, and fixed effects point is the peak of a hill. What were are going to do is draw a topographic map with contour lines to show different elevation regions on that hill.
First, we need to extract the covariance matrix estimated by the model.
# Extract the matrix cov_mat <- VarCorr(m)[["Subject"]] # Strip off some details so that just the useful part is printed attr(cov_mat, "stddev") <- NULL attr(cov_mat, "correlation") <- NULL cov_mat #> (Intercept) Days #> (Intercept) 582.69656 9.89797 #> Days 9.89797 35.03298
The ellipse()
function takes a covariance matrix, a center value, and
quantile/confidence level and returns the points from an oval around the center
at the given confidence level. We create five ellipses for different
quantile levels.
library(ellipse) # Helper function to make a data-frame of ellipse points that # includes the level as a column make_ellipse <- function(cov_mat, center, level) { ellipse(cov_mat, centre = center, level = level) %>% as.data.frame() %>% add_column(level = level) %>% as_tibble() } center <- fixef(m) levels <- c(.1, .3, .5, .7, .9) # Create an ellipse dataframe for each of the levels defined # above and combine them df_ellipse <- levels %>% purrr::map_df(~ make_ellipse(cov_mat, center, level = .x)) %>% rename(Intercept = `(Intercept)`, Slope_Days = Days) df_ellipse #> # A tibble: 500 x 3 #> Intercept Slope_Days level #> <dbl> <dbl> <dbl> #> 1 260.6448 12.43878 0.1 #> 2 260.1491 12.55233 0.1 #> 3 259.6227 12.65743 0.1 #> 4 259.0678 12.75365 0.1 #> 5 258.4867 12.84060 0.1 #> 6 257.8816 12.91793 0.1 #> 7 257.2550 12.98534 0.1 #> 8 256.6094 13.04254 0.1 #> 9 255.9475 13.08931 0.1 #> 10 255.2718 13.12547 0.1 #> # ... with 490 more rows
Then we add them onto our previous plot.
ggplot(df_pulled) + aes(x = Intercept, y = Slope_Days, color = Model) + # Draw contour lines from the distribution of effects geom_path(aes(group = level, color = NULL), data = df_ellipse, linetype = "dashed", color = "grey40") + geom_point(data = df_gravity, size = 5) + geom_point(size = 2) + geom_path(aes(group = Subject, color = NULL), arrow = arrow(length = unit(.02, "npc"))) + theme(legend.position = "bottom") + ggtitle("Topographic map of regression parameters") + xlab("Intercept estimate") + ylab("Slope estimate") + scale_color_brewer(palette = "Dark2")
The ellipses provide a little more information about where the estimates are being pulled. Even if some of the points are not being pulled directly towards the center of gravity, nearlly all of them are being pulled into a higher confidence region.
There are a few tweaks we might consider for this plot. I don’t think the ovals
need to be contained in the plot. The points are more important, and the
plotting boundaries should be set with respect to the points. We can redefine
the limits by using coord_cartesian()
.
last_plot() + coord_cartesian( xlim = range(df_pulled$Intercept), ylim = range(df_pulled$Slope_Days), expand = TRUE)
To go all out :sunglasses:, let’s also label the contours with the confidence levels. I see that the lower left area is relatively free of points, so I can place the labels there. I filter down to just the ellipse points in the bottom 25% of x and y values. That will keep points in that lower left quadrant. Then I find the (x, y) point with the farthest distance from the center as the location for my label. I make it sound so easy but it took a lot of trial and error (including an an attempt to use cosines).
# Euclidean distance contour_dist <- function(xs, ys, center_x, center_y) { x_diff <- (center_x - xs) ^ 2 y_diff <- (center_y - ys) ^ 2 sqrt(x_diff + y_diff) } # Find the point to label in each ellipse. df_label_locations <- df_ellipse %>% group_by(level) %>% filter(Intercept < quantile(Intercept, .25), Slope_Days < quantile(Slope_Days, .25)) %>% # Compute distance from center. mutate(dist = contour_dist(Intercept, Slope_Days, fixef(m)[1], fixef(m)[2])) %>% # Keep smallest values. top_n(-1, wt = dist) %>% ungroup() # Tweak the last plot one more time! last_plot() + geom_text(aes(label = level, color = NULL), data = df_label_locations, nudge_x = .5, nudge_y = .8, size = 3.5, color = "grey40")
Are you feeling satisfied? I feel satisfied.
Plotting lines from a Bayesian mixed effects model
I call myself a Bayesian. Visualizing uncertainty is one of my things here, so I would be remiss if I didn’t also demo how to do some plots using posterior samples.
If we fit a Bayesian model, we can sample from a posterior distribution of partially pooled regression lines. First, we fit the model in RStanARM with weakly informative priors.
library(rstanarm) #> Loading required package: Rcpp #> rstanarm (Version 2.15.3, packaged: 2017-04-29 06:18:44 UTC) #> - Do not expect the default priors to remain the same in future rstanarm versions. #> Thus, R scripts should specify priors explicitly, even if they are just the defaults. #> - For execution on a local, multicore CPU with excess RAM we recommend calling #> options(mc.cores = parallel::detectCores())
b <- stan_glmer( Reaction ~ Days + (Days | Subject), family = gaussian(), data = df_sleep, prior = normal(0, 2), prior_intercept = normal(0, 5), prior_covariance = decov(regularization = 2), prior_aux = cauchy(0, 1))
We get a similar overview as arm::display()
when we print the model.
b #> stan_glmer #> family: gaussian [identity] #> formula: Reaction ~ Days + (Days | Subject) #> ------ #> #> Estimates: #> Median MAD_SD #> (Intercept) 252.4 6.1 #> Days 10.4 1.7 #> sigma 25.7 1.5 #> #> Error terms: #> Groups Name Std.Dev. Corr #> Subject (Intercept) 24 #> Days 7 0.07 #> Residual 26 #> Num. levels: Subject 20 #> #> Sample avg. posterior predictive #> distribution of y (X = xbar): #> Median MAD_SD #> mean_PPD 297.9 2.7 #> #> ------ #> For info on the priors used see help('prior_summary.stanreg').
We have posterior distribution of values now! That means instead of one “center
of gravity” point, we have 4,000 plausible points for our central value. The
center of our former contour plot has its own contour plot. That’s Bayes for
you. We can plot that easily with stat_density_2d()
. We set the coordinate
limits to be the same as the last plot, just so that we don’t exaggerate the
uncertainty around the central point by drawing a gigantic contour surface.
# Get a dataframe: One row per posterior sample df_posterior <- b %>% as.data.frame() %>% as_tibble() ggplot(df_posterior) + aes(x = `(Intercept)`, y = `Days`) + # Calculate the density stat_density_2d(aes(fill = ..level..), geom = "polygon") + ggtitle("Where's the average intercept and slope?") + xlab("Estimate for average intercept") + ylab("Estimate for average slope") + # Use the same coordinate limits as last plot coord_cartesian( xlim = range(df_pulled$Intercept), ylim = range(df_pulled$Slope_Days), expand = TRUE) + guides(fill = "none")
For each participant, we have 4,000 partially-pooled regression lines too, so we can visualize our uncertainty for each participant’s individual regression line.
Let’s finish by drawing a sample of those lines for a faceted plot. We have to do a bunch of data wrangling to get a dataframe with one row per subject per posterior sample.
# For each sample, add the average intercept and average slope values to each # participant's deviation from that average. These yields the intercept and # slope parameters for each participant. df_effects <- df_posterior %>% # Find all the columns with the pattern "b[(Intercept". Add the column # df_posterior$`(Intercept)` to each of those columns. mutate_at( .vars = vars(matches("b\\[\\(Intercept")), .funs = funs(. + df_posterior$`(Intercept)`)) %>% # Again for slope mutate_at( .vars = vars(matches("b\\[Day")), .funs = funs(. + df_posterior$Days)) # Convert to a long format df_long_effects <- df_effects %>% select(matches("b\\[")) %>% rowid_to_column("draw") %>% tidyr::gather(Parameter, Value, -draw) # Extract the effect type and subject number from each parameter name df_long_effects$Type <- df_long_effects$Parameter %>% stringr::str_detect("Intercept") %>% ifelse(., "Intercept", "Slope_Day") df_long_effects$Subject <- df_long_effects$Parameter %>% stringr::str_extract("\\d\\d\\d") df_long_effects <- df_long_effects %>% select(draw, Subject, Effect = Type, Value) # Finally! df_long_effects #> # A tibble: 160,000 x 4 #> draw Subject Effect Value #> <int> <chr> <chr> <dbl> #> 1 1 308 Intercept 236.5760 #> 2 2 308 Intercept 263.6090 #> 3 3 308 Intercept 237.3153 #> 4 4 308 Intercept 247.9472 #> 5 5 308 Intercept 265.3939 #> 6 6 308 Intercept 241.7141 #> 7 7 308 Intercept 255.3133 #> 8 8 308 Intercept 261.2169 #> 9 9 308 Intercept 236.9531 #> 10 10 308 Intercept 264.2734 #> # ... with 159,990 more rows
Now that we have the data in the right shape, we are going randomly choose 50 posterior samples and plot those lines alongside the observed data.
df_samples <- df_long_effects %>% filter(draw %in% sample(1:4000, size = 50)) %>% tidyr::spread(Effect, Value) df_samples #> # A tibble: 1,000 x 4 #> draw Subject Intercept Slope_Day #> * <int> <chr> <dbl> <dbl> #> 1 15 308 266.8775 15.109710 #> 2 15 309 204.5003 4.056596 #> 3 15 310 223.4707 5.505083 #> 4 15 330 255.6470 7.120754 #> 5 15 331 279.8910 6.648727 #> 6 15 332 270.2003 8.059170 #> 7 15 333 283.5603 5.315121 #> 8 15 334 258.0755 11.155002 #> 9 15 335 243.8579 3.091841 #> 10 15 337 284.9078 19.905983 #> # ... with 990 more rows ggplot(df_sleep) + aes(x = Days, y = Reaction) + geom_abline(aes(intercept = Intercept, slope = Slope_Day), data = df_samples, color = "#3366FF", alpha = .1) + geom_point() + facet_wrap("Subject") + scale_x_continuous(breaks = 0:4 * 2) + labs(x = xlab, y = ylab)
For the participants with complete data, the lines pile up and form a narrow band, indicating a low degree of uncertainty. In the final two panels, however, we only have limited data, and the sample of lines fan out and cover many different plausible trajectories.
The uncertainty is more dramatic if we draw a contour plot for each participant—basically, drawing each participants’ mostly likely locations in the landscape of parameter values.
ggplot(df_long_effects %>% tidyr::spread(Effect, Value)) + aes(x = Intercept, y = Slope_Day) + stat_density_2d(aes(fill = ..level..), geom = "polygon") + facet_wrap("Subject") + xlab("Intercept estimate") + ylab("Slope estimate") + theme(legend.position = "bottom") + guides(fill = "none")
For 373 and 374, the contour regions/ink-splats are very tall: A lot of slope values are plausible. The region for 374 is more off center and slightly narrow than that of 373: That extra data point matters.
Funnily enough, this post start a quick write-up of a demo I wrote, but it kind of spiraled out of control. I hope this write-up helps students and users understand mixed-effects models at a more intuitive level.
I had formally learned about these models twice in graduate school. In psychology, we were told to use them if we wanted to make inferences about a larger population of subjects or stimulus items. In educational psychology, we were told to use them to capture the sources of variances in a nested data-set: Kids nested in classrooms nested in schools, etc. It wasn’t until I taught myself Bayesian stats that I learned about third reason to use them: They pool information across different units, providing regularized model estimates. I find this rationale most intuitive. The Gelman and Hill book and Statistical Rethinking both discuss the partial pooling description of these models. (Ooooh, as I added the Rethinking link, I just noticed that I created a ggplot2 version of the plot from the cover of that book. :satisfied:)
R-bloggers.com offers daily e-mail updates about R news and tutorials about learning R and many other topics. Click here if you're looking to post or find an R/data-science job.
Want to share your content on R-bloggers? click here if you have a blog, or here if you don't.